尼古丁的寓意

成瘾性是一个已经有意学究化了的字眼,但还是掩饰不了其中人类的尴尬:我们一有机会就会吹捧意志的自由,声称我们人类因自由心灵而傲立于此物质世界。但,回到座椅,抽出一支香烟,自己可能就知道了,那其实是大话,马克.吐温莞尔一笑:“戒烟其实非常容易,我已经戒了好几百次了。”
人就是一种动物,就是一个物质的结构体系。这点是所谓科学一开始就低调承认的,所以后来人们就更精准地发现了令唱高调者汗颜的,是烟草里面所包含的一种简单的有机分子,尼古丁。
nicotine
尼古丁(nicotine)的分子结构以及它在人脑发生作用的地方

为什么那么一种简单的两个碳原子环组成的分子,具有伤人尊严的能力呢?这样一个问题仍然还是在摆架子,因为反过来我们可以问,人的尊严难道就不能用一个简单分子伤害么?

Comments

人的本质是物质属性

哈哈,这个以前也看过的。包括毒瘾也类似,是毒品中的一种成分与人脑所分泌的一种激素分子式极其接近,取而代之的结果。现在一天一包,我的意志抵不过一包纸烟的威力。

人脑的物质属性: Oxytocin increases trust in humans

Letter
Nature 435, 673-676 (2 June 2005) | doi: 10.1038/nature03701

Oxytocin increases trust in humans
Michael Kosfeld1,5, Markus Heinrichs2,5, Paul J. Zak3, Urs Fischbacher1 and Ernst Fehr1,4

Abstract

Trust pervades human societies 1, 2. Trust is indispensable in friendship, love, families and organizations, and plays a key role in economic exchange and politics3. In the absence of trust among trading partners, market transactions break down. In the absence of trust in a country's institutions and leaders, political legitimacy breaks down. Much recent evidence indicates that trust contributes to economic, political and social success4, 5. Little is known, however, about the biological basis of trust among humans. Here we show that intranasal administration of oxytocin, a neuropeptide that plays a key role in social attachment and affiliation in non-human mammals6, 7, 8, causes a substantial increase in trust among humans, thereby greatly increasing the benefits from social interactions. We also show that the effect of oxytocin on trust is not due to a general increase in the readiness to bear risks. On the contrary, oxytocin specifically affects an individual's willingness to accept social risks arising through interpersonal interactions. These results concur with animal research suggesting an essential role for oxytocin as a biological basis of prosocial approach behaviour.

Trust站得太高了

相对于[fearless|http://krsna.lamost.org/engine/node/368#comment-240],Trust对于人而言实在是一个顶级宏观的概念,因此也很难于收缩为一个明确的概念。
可能降到更低等一些的动物,Trust才好有收缩的可能性,而且,可能是它的反义词更适于被作为一个边界明确的概念,来研究其生物学背景。

Instant waist-shrinking! All coming from above the neck...

Neural Basis of Body Image: How to Lose Inches at the (Perceived) Flick of the Wrist
DOI: 10.1371/journal.pbio.0030439

Published: November 29, 2005

Copyright: © 2005 Public Library of Science. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License, which permits unrestricted use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original work is properly cited.

Citation: (2005) Neural Basis of Body Image: How to Lose Inches at the (Perceived) Flick of the Wrist. PLoS Biol 3(12): e439

--------------------------------------------------------------------------------

Wouldn't it be nice if you could change your body image by placing a vibrating gadget on your wrist? As it happens, you can—though under controlled circumstances. Vibrating skin over the tendon of a joint extensor muscle triggers the vivid sensation that the joint is passively flexing, even though it's not. When the hand is touching the waist, nose, or some other body part, a person can feel the wrist bending and the body part stretching or shrinking—in what's aptly called the Pinocchio illusion.

Vibrations on the skin over a muscle tendon cause the perceptual illusion by exciting sensory nerve endings in the tendon that send signals to brain areas that process touch and motor control, the primary somatosensory cortex and the primary motor cortex. The somatosensory cortex creates neural maps of the body surface, and receives sensory inputs from receptors in the peripheral nervous system. But these peripheral receptors carry no information about the relative size of body parts, and the brain has no specialized neurons to receive such information. The neural map of body size and shape are likely represented in a relative way by integrating signals from the relevant body parts and visual cues. The parietal lobes may play a role, based on reports that patients with parietal cortex injuries imagine changes in the size and shape of various body parts. Still, it's not clear how the brain integrates the relevant information to compute body image.

To investigate the neural correlates of body image, H. Henrik Ehrsson, Eiichi Naito, and their colleagues recruited 24 participants to model the “waist shrinking illusion,” and then scanned their brains with functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI). The authors hypothesized that higher-order somatosensory areas in the parietal cortex would reflect the perceived changes in waist size, and designed the study to isolate illusion-linked brain activity by varying participants' hand position (body contact/no contact, or free) and the vibration site (tendon/skin, or beside the tendon).

After participants experienced each possible combination of hand position and vibration site, they answered “now” when they felt the illusion, and then chose a picture from six different body configurations that best represented their experience. They rated the vividness of the experience, on a scale of zero to nine (absolutely realistic), and then moved their wrists to show what they felt so the authors could measure the angle. At the same time, electromyograms (EMGs) recorded muscle stimulation.

Seven participants did not reliably experience the illusion and so were not scanned. The other 17 participants underwent six experimental trials (two baselines, with hands resting, were added) while their brains were scanned (while lying in the fMRI machine). In three trials, participants' hands lay freely, but supported, by their side without touching the body (tendon free/skin free/rest free). In the other three trials, the palms of the hand were in direct contact with their sides (tendon contact/skin contact/skin free), while a strap allowed them to relax their arms.

During the tendon contact condition, all 17 participants sensed their hands flexing and their waist shrinking. The degree of flexion corresponded to a 28% waist shrinkage. This sensation was vivid, reliable, and quick to start. The EMGs showed no muscle activity in over 70% of the participants, and muscle activity wasn't significantly different in tendon contact and tendon free, confirming that muscle stimulation did not account for the illusion. The brain regions showing most activity during the illusory perception were in the left parietal lobe, within the anterior intraparietal sulcus (a sulcus is an inward fold of the brain) and extending toward the postcentral sulcus.

Participants who reported the strongest shrinking waist illusion also showed the strongest activity in the postcentral sulcus and the anterior left intraparietal cortex—activity that was not observed in participants who felt illusory wrist movement when their hands were not touching their body—confirming a link between these brain regions and the shrinking waist illusion. When the brain receives conflicting sensory information from the vibrated wrists and the sensory inputs of the hands on the waist, the brain recalibrates the relative size of the wrist and shape of the waist, creating the illusion that the waist is shrinking as the hands are bending inward. Altogether, these results suggest that the brain computes body image by integrating signals from the skin, joints, and muscles through hierarchical processing in the somatosensory system. The researchers could elicit this illusion as many times as needed for the fMRI experiment, but there's no indication that a portable device will hit the consumer market anytime soon. —Liza Gross

Syndicate content Syndicate content